National Significance
Today, in this nation, we have both the will and the opportunity
to upgrade education significantly. The will stems from a national
consensus, and a constructive response by government agencies
to that consensus. Political and business leaders, concerned parents,
and typical citizens agree that America needs to improve education
and enhance student achievement. Our educational system has significant
shortcomings as shown in results from cross-national comparisons
(e.g., TIMSS, 1998; Stigler & Heibert, 1999) and by studies
of workplace competence (e. g., SCANS, 1991). Performance in mathematics
and science and among students diagnosed as having disabilities
has caused widespread alarm. There is also broad agreement that
education is the future—not only for our children, but also
for our nation as a whole.
The opportunity to upgrade education comes from extensive progress
in the last several decades on understanding the cognitive processes
that underlie learning. Basic research on learning and memory
now provides a foundation for improving educational practices,
potentially in revolutionary ways. Recent research, for example,
questions the common view that student performance during instruction
indexes learning and validly distinguishes among instructional
practices. Work by Bjork and other researchers has established
that conditions of practice that appear optimal during instruction
can fail to support long-term retention and transfer of knowledge;
whereas, and remarkably, conditions that introduce difficulties
for the learner—slowing the apparent rate of the learning—can
enhance long-term retention and transfer. (Such "desirable
difficulties" (Bjork, 1994, 1999) include spacing rather
than massing study sessions; interleaving rather than blocking
practice on separate topics or tasks; varying how instructional
materials are presented or illustrated; reducing feedback; and
using tests rather than presentations as learning events.)
The opportunity to upgrade instruction also benefits from technology-enhanced
learning environments that enable researchers to test the impact
of these new research findings by consistently varying the conditions
of instruction (e.g. Anderson, et al., 1996; 1997).
Enabled by the OERI Cognition and Student Learning program, we
propose Introducing Desirable Difficulties for Educational Applications
in Science (IDDEAS) to build bridges from the science of cognition
to educational practices. IDDEAS will identify the laboratory-based
principles and phenomena that do and do not generalize to educational
settings and test mechanisms for implementing the principles and
phenomena that do generalize in actual classrooms using technology-based
instruction. IDDEAS requires a partnership of collaborating cognitive
researchers, educational researchers, and classroom teachers who
jointly design and carry out experiments in progressively more
complex educational settings. We propose to form a sustainable
partnership that can build a strong bridge linking the science
of cognition, effective classroom practices, and powerful learning
technologies. If successful, IDDEAS will develop theory-based
principles to guide future instructional designers working in
new contexts.
Abstract
Students’ performance during instruction is commonly viewed
as a measure of learning and a basis for evaluating and selecting
instructional practices. Basic-research findings question that
view: Conditions of practice that appear optimal during instruction
can fail to support long-term retention and transfer of knowledge
and, remarkably, conditions that introduce difficulties for the
learner—and appear to slow the rate of the learning—can
enhance long-term retention and transfer. Such "desirable
difficulties" (Bjork, 1994, 1999) include spacing rather
than massing study sessions; interleaving rather than blocking
practice on separate topics; varying how to-be-learned material
is presented; reducing feedback; and using tests as learning events.
The benefits of desirable difficulties found using simple laboratory
tasks and short retention intervals not only raise concerns about
prevailing educational practices, but also suggest unintuitive
ways to enhance instruction. The present study focuses on whether
such results generalize to realistic educational materials and
contexts. In controlled experiments involving middle-school and
college students, the effectiveness of standard Web-Based Inquiry
Science Environment (WISE, http://wise.berkeley.edu)
projects—on topics such as light propagation, thermal equilibrium,
and science and treatment of malaria—will be contrasted
with the effectiveness of experimental versions that incorporate
selected desirable difficulties.
As a tool for teachers and students, WISE projects can enhance
science education. If successful, this investigation can bridge
the science of cognition and education and provide theoretically
based principles that designers can use to create new materials.
Relevant References
Anderson, M.C., Bjork, R.A., & Bjork, E.L. (1994). Remembering can cause
forgetting: Retrieval dynamics in long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
and Cognition, 20, 1063-1087.
Anderson, M. C., & Spellman, B. A. (1995). On the status of inhibitory
mechanisms in cognition: Memory retrieval as a model case. Psychological
Review, 102, 68-100.
Baddeley, A.D., & Longman, D.J.A. (1978). The
influence of length and frequency of training session on the rate
of learning to type. Ergonomics, 21, 627-635.
Bahrick, H.P. (1979). Maintenance of knowledge:
Questions about memory we forgot to ask. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 296-308.
Battig, W.F. (1956). Facilitation and interference.
In E.A. Bilodeau (Ed.), Acquisition of skill
(pp. 215-244). New York: Academic Press.
Battig,
W. F. (1979). The flexibility of human memory. In L. S. Cermak
& F.I.M. Craik (Eds.), Levels of processing in human memory
(pp. 23-44). Hillsdale, N. J.: Erlbaum.
Becker, Henry Jay. (February 1999). "Internet Use by Teachers: Conditions
of Professional Use and Teacher-Directed Student Use." Center
for Research on Information Technology and Organizations, The
University of California, Irvine and University of Minnesota.
Becker, H., Ravitz, J. & Wong, Y. (1999). Teacher and Teacher-directed
Student Use of Computers and Software. Report #3. Teaching Learning
and Computing: 1998 National Survey. Irvine, CA: University of
California at Irvine, Center for Research in Information Technology
in Organization.
Bjork, R.A. (1994). Memory and metamemory considerations
in the training of human beings. In J. Metcalfe and A. Shimamura
(Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing. (pp.185-205).
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Bjork, R. A. (1999). Assessing our
own competence: Heuristics and illusions. In D. Gopher and A.
Koriat (Eds.), Attention and peformance XVII. Cognitive regulation
of performance: Interaction of theory and application (pp.
435-459). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Bransford, J. D., Brown, A. L., & Cocking, R. R. (Eds.). (1999). How
people learn: Brain, Mind, Experience, and School. Washington,
DC: National Research Council
Bransford, J.D., Sherwood, R.D., Hasselbring, T.S., Kinzer, C.K. & Williams,
S.M. (1990). Anchored instruction: Why we need it and how technology
can help. In Nix, D. & Spiro, R. (Eds.), Cognition, education,
and multimedia. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
Carson, L. M., & Wiegand, R. L. (1979). Motor schema formation and retention
in young children: A test of Schmidt's schema theory. Journal
of Motor Behavior, 11, 247-251.
Catalano, J.F., & Kleiner, B.M. (1984). Distant transfer in coincident
timing as a function of variability of practice. Perceptual
& Motor Skills, 58, 851-856.
Christina, R. W., & Bjork, R. A. (1991). Optimizing long-term retention
and transfer. In D. Druckman & R. A. Bjork (Eds.), In the
mindÕs eye: Enhancing human performance (pp. 23-56). Washington,
DC: National Academy Press.
Davis, E.A. (1998). Scaffolding studentsÕ reflection for science learning.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California at
Berkeley, CA.
Dempster, F. N. (1990). The spacing effect: A case study in the failure
to apply the results of psychological research. American Psychologist,
43, 627-634.
Dempster, F. N. (1996). Distributing and managing the conditions of encoding
and practice. In E. L. Bjork and R. A. Bjork (Eds.), Memory:
Vol. 10. Handbook of Perception and Cognition. (pp. 317-344).
New York: Academic Press.
diSessa, A. (2000). Changing minds: Computers, learning, and literacy. Cambridge,
MA, MIT Press.
Driver, R. (1985). Changing perspectives on science lessons. In N. Bennett,
& C. Desforges (Eds.), Recent advances in classroom research.
British Journal of Psychology Monograph.
Driver, R., Leach, J., Millar, R., & Scott, P. (1996). Young People's
Images of Science. Buckingham, UK: Open University Press.
Edelson, D. C., Gordin, D. N., & Pea, R. D. (1999). Addressing the challenges
of inquiry-based learning through technology and curriculum design.
Journal of the Learning Sciences, 8(3/4),
391-450.
Eylon, B. S. and M. C. Linn (1988). Learning and instruction: An examination
of four research perspectives in science education. Review
of Educational Research 58(3): 251Ð301.
Farr, M. J. (1987). The Long-term Retention of Knowledge and Skills:
A Cognitive and Instructional
Perspective. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Feurzeig, W., & Roberts, N. (Eds.). (1999). Modeling and simulation
in science and mathematics education. New York: Springer.
Gates, A. I. (1917). Archives of Psychology #40
Ghodsian, D., Bjork, R. A., & Benjamin, A. S. (1997).
Evaluating training during training: Obstacles and Opportunities.
In M. A. Quinones and A. Dutta (Eds.), Training for a rapidly
changing workplace: Applications of Psychological Research
(pp. 63-88). Washington DC: American Psychological Association.
Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1980). Analogical problem solving. Cognitive
Psychology, 12, 306-355.
Glenberg, A. M. (1992). Distributed practice effects. In L. R. Squire (Ed.),
Encyclopedia of learning and memory (pp. 138-142). New
York: Macmillan.
Hall, K. G., Domingues, E. & Cavazos, R.
(1992). The effects of contextual interference on extra batting
practice. Unpublished paper.
Hogan, R.M., & Kintsch, W. (1971). Differential effects of study and
test trials on long-term recognition and recall. Journal
of Verbal Learning and Verbal
Behavior, 10, 562-567
Homa, D., & Cultice, J. (1984). Role of feedback, category size, and
stimulus distortion on the acquisition and untilization of ill-defined
categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
and Cognition, 10, 83-94.
Inhelder, B. & Piaget, J. (1958/1972). The growth of logical thinking
from childhood to adolescence. New York: Basic Books.Izawa, 1970).
Izawa,
C. (1970.) Optimal potentiating effects and forgetting-prevention
effects of tests in paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 83, 340-344.
Jacoby, L. L., Bjork, R. A., & Kelley, C. M. (1994). Illusions of comprehensions
and competence. In D. Druckman and R. A. Bjork (Eds.), Learning,
remembering, believing: Enhancing human performance.
(pp.57-80) Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Kafai, Y. B., & Resnick, M. (Eds.). (1996). Constructionism in practice:
Designing, thinking, and learning in a digital world. Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Kerr, R., & Booth, B. (1978). Specific and varied practice of a motor
skill. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
46, 395-401.
Landauer, T. K., & Bjork, R. A. (1978). Optimal rehearsal patterns and
name learning. In M. M. Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes
(Eds.), Practical aspects of memory pp. 625-632). London:
Academic Press.
Langley,
D. J., & Zelaznik, H. N. (1984). The acquisition of time properties
associated with a sequential motor skill. Journal of Motor
Behavior, 16, 275-301.
Lee, T. D., & Genovese, E. D. (1988). Distribution of practice in motor
skill acquisition: Learning and performance effects reconsidered.
Research Quarterly for Exercise and
Sport, 59, 277-287.
Linn, M. C. (1992). The Computer as Learning Partner: Can Computer Tools
Teach Science? In K. Sheingold, L. G. Roberts, and S. M. Malcolm
(Eds.), Technology for Teaching and Learning. Washington,
DC: American Association for the Advancement of Science.
Linn, M. C. (in press). WISE design for lifelong learningÑPivotal Cases.
In Gþrdenfors, P. & Johansson, P. (Eds.), Cognition, Education
and Communication Technology. Mahwah, New Jersey, Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Linn, M. C., & Hsi, S. (2000). Computers, Teachers, Peers: Science Learning
Partners. Mahwah: Erlbaum.
Linn,
M. C. (2000). "Designing the Knowledge Integration Environment:
The partnership inquiry process." International Journal
of Science Education, Special Issue (22): 781-796.
Linn, M.C. & Songer, N.B. (1991). Teaching thermodynamics to middle
school students: What are appropriate cognitive demands? Journal
of Research in Science Teaching, 28, 885-918.
Mannes S.M., & Kintsch, W. (1987). Knowledge organization and text organization.
Cognition and Instruction, 4, 91-115.
Morris, C.D., Bransford, J.D., & Franks, J.J. (1977). Levels of processing
versus transfer appropriate processing. Journal of
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,
16, 519-533.
National Research Countil (1999). High Stakes: Testing for Tracking, Promotion
and Graduation. Washington, D.C.
Pea, R. D. & Gomez, L. (1993). Distributed multimedia learning environments:
The Collaborative Visualization Project. Communications of the
ACM, 36(5), 60-63.
Piaget, J. (1971). Structuralism. New York: Harper and Row.
Rea, C.P., & Modigliani, V. (1985). The effect of expanded versus massed
practice on the retention of multiplication facts and spelling
lists. Human Learning, 4, 11-18.
Reder, L. M., Charney, D. H. & Morgan, K. I. (1986). The role of elaborations
in learning a skill from an instructional text. Memory and
Cognition, 14, 64-78.
Reder, L. M., & Klatzky, R. L. (1994). Transfer: Training for performance.
In D. Druckman and R. A. Bjork (Eds.), Learning, remembering,
believing: Enhancing
human performance (pp. 25-56). Washington, DC: National Academy
Press.
Reiser, B.J., Kimberg, D.Y., Lovett, M.C., & Ranney, M. (1992). Knowledge
representation and explanation in GIL, an intelligent tutor for
programming. In J.H. Larkin & R.W. Chabay, Computer-assisted
instruction and intelligent tutoring systems: Shared goals and
complimentary approaches. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
Scardamalia, M., & Bereiter, C. (1991). Higher levels of agency for
children in knowledge building: A challenge for the design of
new knowledge media. Journal of the Learning Sciences,
1(1), 37-68.
Schmidt, R.A. (1991). Frequent augmented feedback
can degrade learning: Evidence and interpretations. In G.E. Stelmach
& J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor
neuroscience (pp. 59-75). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Schmidt, R. A., & Bjork, R. A. (1992). New conceptualizations
of practice: Common principles in three paradigms suggest new
concepts for training. Psychological Science, 3,
207-217.
Schmidt, R.A., Young, D.E., Swinnen, S., &
Shapiro, D.C. (1989). Summary knowledge of results for skill acquisition:
Support for the guidance hypothesis. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
and Cognition, 15, 352-359.
Shea, J.B., & Morgan, R.L. (1979) Contextual
interference effects on the acquisition, retention, and transfer
of a motor skill. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Learning and Memory,
5, 179-187.
Simon, D. A., & Bjork, R. A. (2001). Metacognition in
motor learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, and Cognition, 27, 907-912.
Slotta, J.D., Chi, M.T.H. & Joram, E. (1995). Assessing the ontological
nature of conceptual physics: A contrast of experts and novices.
Cognition and Instruction.
Smith, S. M, Glenberg, A. M., & Bjork, R. A. (1978).
Environmental context and human memory. Memory & Cognition,
6, 342-353. (
Smith, S. M., & Rothkopf, E. Z. (1984). Contextual enrichment
and distribution of practice in the classroom. Cognition
and Instruction, 1(3), 341-358.
Songer, N. B. (1996). Exploring learning opportunities in coordinated network-enhanced
classrooms: A case of kids as global scientists. The Journal
of the Learning Sciences, 5,
297-327.
Slotta, J. & Linn, M.C. (2000). The Knowledge Integration Environment:
Helping students use the Internet effectively. In M.J. Jacobson
& R.Kozma (Eds.), Innovations in Science and Mathematics
Education: Advanced Designs for Technologies of Learning (pp.
193-226). Mahwah: Erlbaum.
Vanderbilt, Cognition and Technology Group. (1997). The Jasper Project:
Lessons in curriculum, instruction, assessment, and professional
development: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA, MIT
Press.
White, B.Y. & Frederiksen, J.R. (1998). Inquiry, modeling, and metacognition:
Making science accessible to students. Cognition and Instruction,
16, 3-118.
Winstein, C.J., & Schmidt, R.A. (1990). Reduced
frequency of knowledge of results enhances motor skill learning.
Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16,
677-691.